Deutsch
 
Hilfe Datenschutzhinweis Impressum
  DetailsucheBrowse

Datensatz

DATENSATZ AKTIONENEXPORT
  What is the functional role of adult neurogenesis in the hippocampus?

Wiskott, L., Rasch, M., & Kempermann, G. (2005). What is the functional role of adult neurogenesis in the hippocampus?. Poster presented at Computational and Systems Neuroscience Meeting (COSYNE 2005), Salt Lake City, UT, USA.

Item is

Externe Referenzen

einblenden:

Urheber

einblenden:
ausblenden:
 Urheber:
Wiskott, L, Autor
Rasch, MJ1, Autor           
Kempermann, G, Autor
Affiliations:
1Department Physiology of Cognitive Processes, Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics, Max Planck Society, ou_1497798              

Inhalt

einblenden:
ausblenden:
Schlagwörter: -
 Zusammenfassung: The hippocampus is a brain structure that is instrumental for episodic memory, i.e. for memorizing facts and events. It is often thought to be an intermediate memory that can store new input patterns quickly, which subsequently get transferred into more permanent cortical memory. The entorhinal cortex serves as an interface between hippocampus and other cortical areas. Within the hippocampal formation the different substructures form a loop: entorhinal cortex (layers II/III) - dentate gyrus - CA3 - CA1 - subiculum - entorhinal cortex (layers V/VI). Because of its recurrent connectivity, CA3 is thought to be the actual memory. Dentate gyrus would then be an encoding network, preparing the input patterns for storage in CA3; CA1 and subiculum would perform the decoding to reconstruct the stored patterns in entorhinal cortex. The dentate gyrus is special in that it generates new neurons throughout life, a phenomenon referred to as adult neurogenesis. Why does adult neurogenesis occur in the dentate gyrus and not in any of the other structures? Assume the dentate gyrus adapts to the environment the animal lives in in order to optimize its encoding for the input-pattern distribution encountered in this environment. If the animal moves to another environment, new adaptation takes place and the dentate gyrus is faces with the problem of catastrophic interference. As a new encoding is learned the old encoding degrades quickly and as a consequence old patterns could not be addressed and retrieved from the CA3-memory. In artificial neural networks catastrophic interference is usually avoided by interleaved training, i.e. the training patterns are presented repeatedly in an alternating fashion. However, this is not possible in real life, because many patterns occur only once. How can the dentate gyrus solve this problem? We hypothesize that new neurons are the solution to this problem. If the dentate gyrus keeps old neurons and their connections fixed but adds new neurons that are plastic, it can adapt to qualitatively new input patterns but at the same time maintain the encoding capabilities for old patterns. Note that new neurons are required only for qualitatively new patterns and not for new patterns that belong to the old input distribution, because we assume the encoding to be characteristic for a distribution and not for individual patterns. As a proof of principle we have simulated a linear auto-encoder network modelling the loop within the hippocampal formation. We assume that the animal first lives in environment A with a certain input-pattern statistics, then moves to a new environment B with a different input-pattern statistics, and finally returns to environment A. We assume that the animal has time to adapt to environment A and then B, but when it returns to A we only test the performance without giving the time for new adaptation. We also assume that the decoding (CA1/subiculum) stays plastic and can adapt to environment A and B in any case (but not when the animal returns to A). We have considered three different scenarios: (a) No DG-adaptation: The dentate gyrus adapts to environment A and keeps the synaptic weights fixed after that. No adaptation to environment B occurs. (b) Neurogenesis: The dentate gyrus starts with fewer units and first adapts to environment A. In environment B the old units and connections are fixed but a few new units are added and used to adapt to the new input-pattern distribution. (c) Full adaptation: The dentate gyrus always fully adapts to the current environment, first A then B. In the simulations we find that the networks always perform reasonably well on the pattern distributions they are adapted to. However, in scenario (a) the performance is poor in environment B, because although the decoding can adapt to environment B the encoding is still optimal for A and misses important dimensions of B. Performance is also poor when the animal returns to A, because the decoding has adapted to B. In scenario (c) performance is particularly poor when the animal returns to environment A, because the network is then fully adapted to B. Only in scenario (b) is the effect of catastrophic interference largely avoided and the performance good in environments A and B and also as the animal returns to A. Our model is consistent with a number of anatomical and physiological facts: New neurons are found to be more plastic than old ones as required by our model. Since new units are only required for qualitatively new input patterns, there is decreasing need for neurogenesis with age, because the animal has more and more experience and encounters fewer and fewer qualitatively new stimuli. This is consistent with the decrease in neurogenesis observed experimentally. A relatively small number of newly added units can have a large effect, since only missing dimensions have to be newly encoded. This is consistent with the relatively low level of neurogenesis of 30 new neurons in mice over the whole lifetime. Since the generation of new neurons takes weeks but the demand for new neurons can be on a much shorter time scale when the animal changes its environment, it is reasonable that new neurons are generated all the time to have some in stock when needed. New neurons not needed die after some time. This is what is found experimentally. The level of neurogenesis is regulated by rather unspecific factors such as physical activity or hunger. It is clear that if new neurons are only needed if qualitatively new input-pattern distributions are encountered, there are no specific factors that would be available earlyenough. Thus the unspecific factors might actually be fairly descent predictors for the need of new neurons, since hunger andrunning fosters exploration of new environments. In summary we hypothesize that adult neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus helps to solve the problem of catastrophic interference when an animal adapts to new environments. Our network simulations confirm that adding new neurons can reduce the effect of catastrophic interference significantly. The model is also qualitatively consistent with a number of anatomical and physiological facts about adult neurogenesis. See http://cogprints.org/4012/ for more information.

Details

einblenden:
ausblenden:
Sprache(n):
 Datum: 2005-03
 Publikationsstatus: Erschienen
 Seiten: -
 Ort, Verlag, Ausgabe: -
 Inhaltsverzeichnis: -
 Art der Begutachtung: -
 Identifikatoren: URI: http://www.cosyne.org/c/index.php?title=Cosyne_05
BibTex Citekey: 4703
 Art des Abschluß: -

Veranstaltung

einblenden:
ausblenden:
Titel: Computational and Systems Neuroscience Meeting (COSYNE 2005)
Veranstaltungsort: Salt Lake City, UT, USA
Start-/Enddatum: -

Entscheidung

einblenden:

Projektinformation

einblenden:

Quelle

einblenden: